A intenção de prevenção e de gastos futuros durante a COVID-19: um estudo considerando a tomada de decisões sob risco
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.5585/remark.v19i4.18516Palavras-chave:
Consumo, Covid-19, Intenção, Risco, Modelagem de Equações Estruturais.Resumo
Objetivo: O objetivo principal do estudo foi analisar os aspectos que influenciam na intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19, o objetivo secundário foi analisar a influência da intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19 na intenção de gastos futuros.
Método: Para testar o modelo proposto, foi feito um levantamento com 402 pessoas. O modelo foi analisado por meio de Modelagem de Equações Estruturais baseada em variâncias (PLS-SEM).
Originalidade/Relevância: Um modelo foi proposto, testado e validado para analisar a intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19.
Resultados: Os resultados indicam que 49,4% da variância da intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19 é explicada pelo modelo proposto, sendo gravidade percebida e norma subjetiva antecessores de crença de autoeficácia (R2=0,326), que por sua vez é antecessor da intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19. Ademais, observou-se uma leve influência da gravidade percebida na intenção de gastos futuros (R2=0,008; β = 0,104; p < 0,01).
Contribuições teóricas: As principais contribuições deste estudo foram: identificar um modelo com relevante capacidade explicativa da intenção de se prevenir da Covid-19; demonstrar novas relações entre construtos ainda não testados na literatura, especificamente a influência da norma subjetiva na crença de autoeficácia e desta na intenção de gastos futuros; e demonstrar que alguns aspectos sociodemográficos influenciam a gravidade percebida, a crença de autoeficácia e a intenção de se prevenir de Covid-19.
Downloads
Referências
Agência Brasil. (2020). Segunda onda da Covid-19 pode levar PIB do Brasil a cair 9,1%. Recovered from https://agenciabrasil.ebc.com.br/economia/noticia/2020-06/segunda-onda-da-Covid-19-pode-levar-pib-do-brasil-cair-91.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 50(2), 179-211.
Aktipis, C. A., & Kurzban, R. (2004). Is Homo economicus extinct? Vernon Smith, Daniel Kahneman and the evolutionary perspective. In R. Koppl (Ed.), Advances in Austrian economics, 7, 135-153.
Allais, M. (1953). Le comportement de l’homme rationnel devant le risque: Critique des postulats et axiomes de l’école Américaine [Rational human behavior regarding risk: A critique of axioms of the American school]. Econometrica, 21, 503-546.
Babbie, E. (2003). Métodos de pesquisas de survey. Belo Horizonte: Editora UFMG.
Bandura, A. (2009). Cultivate self-efficacy for personal and organizational effectiveness. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), Handbook of principles of organization behavior (2a ed., pp. 179-200). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman.
Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of personality. In L. Pervin & O. John (eds.), Handbook of Personality (2a ed., pp. 154-196). New York: Guilford.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1-26.
Barrett, H. C., & Fiddick, L. (1999). Evolution and risky decisions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 251-252.
Berg, N., & Gigerenzer, G. (2010). As-if behavioral economics: Neoclassical economics in disguise? History of Economic Ideas, 18, 133-166.
Boletim Focus. (2020). Focus Relatório de Mercado. Recovered from https://www.bcb.gov.br/publicacoes/focus/19062020.
Bontempo, R., Bottom, W., & Weber, E. (1997). Cross-cultural differences in risk perception: A model based approach. Risk Analysis, 17, 479-488.
Brandstätter, E., Gigerenzer, G., & Hertwig, R. (2006). The priority heuristic: Making choices without trade-offs. Psychological Review, 113, 409-432.
Brewer, N., Chapman, G. B., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Mccaul, K. D., & Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Meta-Analysis of the Relationship Between Risk Perception and Health Behavior: The Example of Vaccination. Health Psychology, 26(2), 136-145.
Burns, A., Mensbrugghe D., & Timmer H. (2006). Evaluating the Economic Consequences of Avian Influenza, in Global Development Finance. Washington: World Bank.
Chauhan, V., & Shah, H. (2020). An Empirical Analysis into Sentiments, Media Consumption Habits, and Consumer Behaviour during the Coronavirus (COVID-19) Outbreak. Purakala UGC Care Journal, 31(20).
Chiou, J. (1998). The effects of attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control on consumers’ purchase intentions: the moderating effects of product knowledge and attention to social comparison information. Proceedings of the National Science Council, 9(2). Republic of China. 298-308.
Cho, H., & Lee, J. S. (2015). The influence of self-efficacy, subjective norms, and risk perception on behavioral intentions related to the H1N1 flu pandemic: A comparison between Korea and the US. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 18(4), 1-32.
Chronopoulos, D. K., Lukas, M., & Wilson, J. O. (2020). Consumer Spending Responses to the COVID-19 Pandemic: An Assessment of Great Britain. Available at SSRN 3586723.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.
Deloitte. (2020). Covid-19 e os impactos nos setores: Um olhar atento às projeções futuras e à evolução dos negócios durante a pandemia. Recovered from https://www2.deloitte.com/br/pt/pages/about-deloitte/articles/combate-covid-setores.html.
Ellsburg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 75, 643-669.
Floyd, D. L., Prentice‐Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (2000). A meta‐analysis of research on protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(2), 407-429.
Fiocruz. (2020). Observatório COVID-19. Recovered from https://portal.fiocruz.br/impactos-sociais-economicos-culturais-e-politicos-da-pandemia.
Gibbons, F. X., Helweg-Larsen, M., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Prevalence estimates and adolescent risk behavior: Cross-cultural differences in social influence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 107-121.
Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision-making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 451-482.
Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R., & Pachur, T. (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Gorbalenya, A., Baker, S., Baric, R., Groot, R., Drosten, C., Gulyaeva, A., & Penzar, D. (2020). Severe acute respiratory syndrome-related coronavirus: The species and its viruses–a statement of the Coronavirus Study Group. Recuperado de https://digital.csic.es/bitstream/10261/212994/1/Severe%20acute_Gorbalenya.pdf.
Hair Jr., J. F., Gabriel, M. L. D. S., Silva, D., & Braga Jr., S. S. B. (2019). Development and validation of attitudes measurement scales: fundamental and practical aspects. RAUSP Management Journal, 54(4), 490-507.
Hair Jr, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2017). A primer on partial least squares structural modeling (PLS-SEM) (2a ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Hair Jr.; J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, Ronald L. (2009). Análise Multivariada de Dados (6a ed.) Porto Alegre: Bookman.
Hall, M. C., Prayag, G., Fieger, P., & Dyason, D. (2020). Beyond panic buying: consumption displacement and COVID-19. Journal of Service Management, DOI 10.1108/JOSM-05-2020-0151.
IstoÉ Dinheiro. (2020). COVID-19 e as mudanças forçadas nos hábitos de consumo. Recovered from https://www.istoedinheiro.com.br/Covid-19-e-as-mudancas-forcadas-nos-habitos-de-consumo/.
Johnson, E. J., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., & Willemsen, M. C. (2008). Process models deserve process data: Comment on Brandstätter, Gigerenzer, and Hertwig. Psychological Review, 115, 263-272.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 313-327.
Katsikopoulos, K. V., & Gigerenzer, G. (2008). One-reason decision-making: Modeling violations of expected utility theory. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37, 35-56.
Keogh-Brown, M. R., & Smith, R. D. (2008). The Economic Impact of SARS: How Does the Reality Match the Predictions? Health Policy, 88(1), 110-120
Kline, R. B. (2019). Becoming a behavioral science researcher: a guide to producing research that matters. (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Koonin, L. M. (2020). Novel coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak: Now is the time to refresh pandemic plans. Journal of Business Continuity & Emergency Planning, 13(4) 1-15.
Kostova D., Cassell C. H., Redd J. T., Williams D. E., Singh T., Martel L. D., & Bunnell R. E. (2019). Long-distance effects of epidemics: Assessing the link between the 2014 West Africa Ebola outbreak and U.S. exports and employment, Health Economics, 28, 1248-1261.
Lee, C., & Green, R. T. (1991). Crosscultural examination of the Fishbein behavioral intentions model. Journal of International Business Studies, 22, 289-305.
Martin, A., Markhvida, M., Hallegatte, S., & Walsh, B. (2020). Socio-economic impacts of COVID-19 on household consumption and poverty. Economics of Disasters and Climate Change, 4(3), 453-479.
Mckinsey. (2020). COVID-19: Implications for business. Recovered from https://www.mckinsey.com/business-functions/risk/our-insights/Covid-19-implications-for-business#.
Ministério da Saúde. (2020a). Brasil confirma primeiro caso da doença. Recovered from https://www.saude.gov.br/noticias/agencia-saude/46435-brasil-confirma-primeiro-caso-de-novo-coronavirus.
Ministério da Saúde. (2020b). Painel Coronavírus. Recovered from https://covid.saude.gov.br/.
Mishra, S. (2014). Decision-making under risk: Integrating perspectives from biology, economics, and psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18, 280-307.
Nielsen. (2020). Life Beyond COVID-19: What manufacturers and retailers must now prepare for. Recovered from https://www.nielsen.com/ch/en/insights/article/2020/life-beyond-Covid-19/.
Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). The Assessment of Reliability. Psychometric Theory, 3, 248-292.
OMS – Organização Mundial da Saúde. (2020). WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. Recovered from <https://covid19.who.int/.
Pedersen, C. L., & Ritter, T. (2020). Preparing your business for a postpandemic world. Harvard Busisnes Review. Recovered from:https://hbr.org/2020/04/preparing-your-business-for-a-post-pandemic-world.
Pereira, V., & Dias, P. C. (2018). Atitudes e Consumo de Cannabis em Estudantes do Ensino Geral e Vocacional. Psicologia, Saúde & Doenças, 19(3), 535-549.
Putrevu, S., & Lord, K. R. (1994). Comparative and Noncomparative Advertising: Attitudinal Effects under Cognitive and Affective Involvement Conditions. Journal of Advertising, 23(2), 77-91.
Rassy, D., & Smith, R. (2013). The Economic Impact of H1N1 on Mexico's Tourist and Pork Sectors. Health Economics, 22, 824-834.
Regalado-Pezúa, O., Guerrero, C., Ruiz, J., & Morales, J. (2016). Influence of non-economic factors in the use of personal care products: the case of male Peruvian consumer. Universidad & Empresa, 18(31), 121-138.
Relihan, L., Ward, M., Wheat, C. W., & Farrell, D. (2020). The early impact of COVID-19 on local commerce: changes in spend across neighborhoods and online. Covid Economics.
Ringle, C., Silva, D., & Bido, D. S. (2014). Modelagem de equações estruturais com utilização do Smartpls. Revista Brasileira de Marketing, 13(2), 54-71.
Rode, C., & Wang, X. T. (2000). Risk-sensitive decision-making examined within an evolutionary framework. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 926-939.
Salanova, M., Grau, R., & Martínez, I. (2006). Job Demands and coping behaviours: the moderating role of professional self-efficacy. Psychology in Spain, 10, 1-7.
Sharma, P., Leung, T. Y., Kingshott, R. P., Davcik, N. S., & Cardinali, S. (2020). Managing uncertainty during a global pandemic: An international business perspective. Journal of Business Research, 116, 188-192.
Social Miner. (2020). Tendências de consumo num mundo pós-Covid-19. Recovered from http://blog.socialminer.com/people-marketing/tendencias-de-consumo-num-mundo-pos-Covid-19
Starmer, C. (2000). Developments in non-expected utility theory: The hunt for a descriptive theory of choice under risk. Journal of Economic Literature, 38, 332-382.
Todd, P. M. (2000). The ecological rationality of mechanisms evolved to make up minds. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 940-956.
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2012). Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Précis of Simple heuristics that make us smart. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 727-780.
Venkatesan, R., Kumar, V., & Ravishanker, N. (2007). Multichannel shopping: causes and consequences. Journal of Marketing, 71(2), 114-132.
Wang, Y., Hong, A., Li, X., & Gao, J. (2020). Marketing innovations during a global crisis: A study of China firms’ response to COVID-19. Journal of Business Research, 116, 214-220.
Winterhalder, B., Lu, F., & Tucker, B. (1999). Risk-sensitive adaptive tactics: Models and evidence from subsistence studies in biology and anthropology. Journal of Archaeological Research, 7, 301-347.
Wu, G., Zhang, J., & Gonzalez, R. (2004). Decision under risk. In D. Koehler; N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 399-423). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Downloads
Publicado
Como Citar
Edição
Seção
Licença
Copyright (c) 2020 André Francisco Alcântara Fagundes, Rejane Alexandrina Domingues Pereira do Prado, Marcelo Luiz Dias da Silva Gabriel, Sérgio Luiz do Amaral Moretti

Este trabalho está licenciado sob uma licença Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
- Resumo 1057
- PDF (English) 620